Scholar Works Scholar Works
M.S.Ed. in Educational Leadership Research
Projects
Graduate Works
5-2020
Multiplication Fact Fluency and Multiple Intelligences in a Third Multiplication Fact Fluency and Multiple Intelligences in a Third
Grade Classroom Grade Classroom
Julie A. Nugent
Follow this and additional works at: https://scholarworks.umf.maine.edu/ed_leadership_projects
Part of the Educational Leadership Commons
1
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Multiplication Fact Fluency and Multiple Intelligences in a Third Grade Classroom
Julie A. Nugent
University of Maine at Farmington
2
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Abstract
The purpose of this quasi-experimental study was to examine if teaching students their
multiplication facts in a way that utilized their multiple intelligence strength would be a more
effective way of teaching students their multiplication facts than having students learn their
multiplication facts through rote memorization only. All four groups who received a teaching of
multiplication facts intervention made gains in learning their multiplication facts. The three
multiple intelligence groups did make more gains in learning more facts, than the one group who
only learned their facts through rote memorization. However, the rote memorization group was
able to give more facts automatically in the post assessment when compared with the other three
multiple intelligence groups.
3
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Review of Literature
How Do Children Best Learn Math Facts ?: A Literature Review
Math fact fluency in the elementary years is extremely important. Students need to be
fluent in knowing their addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division facts. Fluent means
they can accurately and automatically recall the answers to all their math facts. Burns et al.
(2015) found that students who were fluent in their math facts, had more time and energy to
spend on the demands of problem solving. If students are unable to master their math facts, they
have to work much harder and longer on more complex math problems. This can cause
frustration and makes more complex math problems more difficult to solve. It can even lead to
children developing a dislike for math (Boaler 2015).
Review of Literature
Teaching Children Math Facts Through the Use of Technology
Musti-Rao and Plait (2015) found that in order to improve learning for all students and
increase instructional efficiency, teachers need interventions that can be applied class wide or to
large groups of students, but at the same time differentiate instruction by providing an
appropriate level of challenge for each student. If a school has computer devices for each
student, that computer device may be used to give students that individualized instruction.
Multiple studies involved children learning their facts using technology. Researchers found that
children really enjoyed using technology, such as an iPad or laptop, to learn their math facts.
(Berrett & Carter, 2018; Caviola, Gerotto, & Mammarella, 2015; Musti-Rao, & Plati, 2015;
Nelson, Burnes, Kanive, & Ysseldyke, 2013) In multiple studies, children were asked to take a
pre-test to see how many math facts they knew, they practiced their math facts using a computer
app for a certain period of time, and then took a post test to see how many more math facts they
4
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
knew after using the app. Berrett and Carter (2018) found that using technology, many of the
recommended and effective practices intended to improve math fact fluency such as modeling,
drill and practice, immediate and regular feedback, and adaptive individualized presentation
were able to be accomplished by the technology device.
Teachers have reported they liked using technology because it could be differentiated for
each student, it was easy to use, and the children enjoyed the practice time. Nelson, Burns,
Kanieve, and Ysseldyke (2013) found that the inherent flexibility of technology-based practice
interventions allows for individuals, small group, or classroom application- something that
stands in contrast to similar interventions that may require more time and personnel as the
number of students increase. Students were able to make more responses using the technology
involved with iPads or desktops and students assigned to the iPad practice-base intervention
groups tended to produce higher fluency scores relative to students assigned to the control group.
Shobana and Plati (2015) found that students showed a steady increase in the number of
math facts they practiced during each of the intervention sessions, making twice as many
responses in the iPad intervention when compared to a Detect, Probe, Repeat, (DPR) practice
worksheet. Students liked being able to tap a screen or click an answer more than writing out
their facts on a paper with a pencil. Because of these factors, students who learned their math
facts using technology were able to learn more math facts.
Teaching Children Math Facts Rote Method
Another way children can learn math facts is through repeated practice using a paper and
pencil procedure. Several studies reviewed involved children learning their math facts through
rote memorization of some kind. There seem to be many paper and pencil methods of teaching
math facts. A pretest is given before the intervention begins, the paper pencil intervention is
5
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
given, and then a post test is given to track student progress. All the paper pencil methods
involve the child writing out the math facts as a way of learning them. Researchers in these
studies have shown that repeated practice using paper and pencil practice, writing out math facts
repeatedly, helps students achieve fluency, and the ability to retain what they have learned. Some
math facts require more repetition to learn than do others, and students at younger grades or with
math difficulties may require more repetition that students in older graders or with higher math
skills. (Burns, Nelson, Ysseldyke, and Kanive, 2015; Schutte, et al., 2015)
Teaching Children Math Facts by Playing Games
Another way to teach children math facts is by having them repeatedly play math games
involving math facts. In this way, children are learning their math facts in a way that is not timed
and is not done by having students memorize facts by either repeatedly writing them or typing
them on a computer screen like the previous two methods. Boaler (2015) found that
mathematics facts are important, but the memorization of math facts through times table
repetition practice and timed testing is unnecessary and causes math anxiety. By having children
play math fact games, they develop number sense. According to Boaler (2015), number sense is
a person’s ability to use and understand numbers. The researchers of these studies provided
various games that focused on understanding the mathematical operations rather than just
memorizing the facts. The games often included hands on materials and discussions. Godfrey
and Stone (2013) found when students played math fact fluency games, students focused on
higher-level thinking strategies and by discussing them with a partner, this caused their fluency
and number sense to increase.
6
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Conclusion
Poncy, Fontenelle, and Skinner (2013) found that many students in our country do not
know their math facts, addition, subtraction, multiplication, and division. This lack of math fact
mastery has led to students struggling with more complex math problems and math frustration.
The goal of this literature review was to research what is the best way for students to learn their
math facts. Through this literature review, three main ways of teaching math facts have become
clear. The first and second ways are by repeated practice whether it is on a computer or by paper
and pencil. The third way is by playing math fact math games that involve students developing a
strong number sense through discussion and hands on materials. All three ways work in the
research. The repeated practice using computer technology seems to be the easiest for teachers to
use, the most differentiated, and the favorite among students. The repeated rote practice also
worked, was very popular, and can be done in various ways. The way of playing math games
seems to focus more on students understanding the math facts, retention of math facts and not on
just memorization of facts.
These methods were all shown to work, but none of them looked into the fact that
children are all individuals with individual multiple intelligence strengths that could be used to
help them learn. Multiple Intelligence is way of looking at human intelligence. In this view of
intelligence, Gardner (2011) found that humans are intelligent in many different ways, and each
type of intelligence is correlated to a specific area of the brain and corresponds with certain
personal skills and preferences. Teachers have always known from their own teaching
experiences, that students learn in different ways, and Gardner’s Multiple Intelligence (MI)
Theory supports their own classroom experiences. Traditionally, schools have been designed for
students who are mathematical and linguistic, not for students who are artistic, musical or
7
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
kinesthetic learners. If educators can recognize that students learn in different ways, they can
design learning activities that involve student strengths. The result of this will be more
engagement and, therefore, more learning for students.
Research Purpose/Question
The purpose of this study was to examine if teaching students their multiplication facts in
a way that utilized their multiple intelligence strength would be a more effective way of teaching
students their multiplication facts. Does this method of teaching students by utilizing their
multiple intelligence strength, improve a student’s ability to learn their multiplication facts more
effectively than a more traditional rote memorization method?
Methods
This was a quasi-experimental design study. Specifically, a group of eight and nine-year-
old children in a third-grade classroom were surveyed to find each student’s multiple
intelligence strength. The survey was found and shown to the students’ third-grade classroom
teacher to make sure it was age appropriate or if she thought they would have any trouble
reading it. It was reviewed and edited to remove formal language, so the students were better
able to understand it (See appendix A). Help was given to any students who needed help reading
any of it when the survey was given. Each of the statements on the survey were read aloud to the
students one at a time so that they could ask any clarifying questions they had. The students
wrote in their numbers for each statement on the survey. They filled in each number for each
statement one at a time. Their classroom teacher had reported that they do a zero-five scale each
day for their daily behavior goal, so they were very familiar with the zero-five system of rating
things that was used in the survey.
8
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
From that survey, three multi-intelligence-based groups were formed. The multiple
intelligence strength groups were: musical-rhythmic(five students), visual-spatial(four students),
and body-kinesthetic group(four students). All eight multi-intelligence groups were not formed
because not all of them fit with teaching multiplication facts. A fourth group was also formed
that would learn their math facts only through rote memorization (See appendix C for an
example of the rote memorization worksheets).
A math running record was given to each of the students in the class before the
intervention was started, to determine how many multiplication facts they already knew and how
many they didn’t know. See appendix B for the multiplication running record sheet. The students
were then separated into the four groups(three multi-intelligence groups and one rote
memorization group). Each group received the math intervention for ten minutes per day during
their math block period, Monday-Friday. Learning multiplication facts is a required standard for
third grade in this school, so learning multiplication facts was something the class would
normally be doing even if this intervention were not being done.
During the first two weeks of his experiment, all four groups of students started learning
their multiplication facts in a very traditional manner, rote memorization. Specifically, all four
groups were given multiplication fact practice worksheets for them to complete and the students
practiced their multiplication facts by writing out their multiplication facts repeatedly. See
appendix C for multiplication worksheet example. How many math facts each student learned
was tracked by giving them a formative, untimed, mixed, biweekly, multiplication facts quiz.
The biweekly assessment was one the classroom teacher routinely used and took less than 3
minutes to complete. The results of this biweekly quiz were used to form instruction for the next
week and to see if there was a difference in the amount of facts learned between the multiple
9
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
intelligence groups and the rote memorization group. This was done throughout the experiment.
See appendix D for the math facts quiz.
After two weeks of having all students do the repeated rote learning of multiplication
facts, three of the four groups started learning their multiplication facts through the multiple
intelligence strength math interventions. The study was started in this way so that students in the
three multiple intelligence groups could see how different it was to learn their facts from just
writing them out to the activities they were doing in the multiple intelligence groups. Examples
of how students were taught their multiplication facts utilizing the students’ multiple intelligence
strength are shown in Table 1.
Table 1
Multiple Intelligence Group Activities
Visual-Spatial
Drew and created arrays, drew groups of objects, made area models, and
wrote out repeated addition for each multiplication fact in all of the times
tables to get a good visual for each fact.
Musical-Rhythmic
Watched multiplication rap videos for each times table and the group was
given a multiplication song sheet that went with a well know song. Each
day the group watched the multiplication rap video and also sang the
multiplication fact song together multiple times.
Body-Kinesthetic
Built each fact with hands on manipulatives such as Legos and adding
groups of marbles into egg carton containers.
Wrote out their facts on pieces of card stock which they laid out in the
hallway like a hopscotch game. They then hopped from fact to fact saying
the fact as they went. They also put their multiplication fact cards in a line
next to each other and hopped next to each other saying their facts as they
hopped on their cards as a race to see who could say their facts first.
Rote Memorization Filled in one multiplication fact practice sheet each day in class.
10
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Then after the six-week intervention period, a multiplication running record was again
given to each student in the class to see what growth each student had made in learning their
multiplication facts as a result of this intervention.
The results of the students who were taught their math facts in a way that utilized their
multiple intelligence strength was compared to the students who continued to learn their
multiplication facts through rote multiplication only. This was done to see if the multiple
intelligence groups had more growth than the group that learned their multiplication facts by just
writing out their multiplication facts. Comparisons were made to see how many multiplication
facts each student learned individually, and then each group learned, before and after the math
intervention was done. How many multiplication facts each individual student, and then each
group learned to automaticity, was also completed.
Site
This study took place in York County in Southern Maine at Waterboro Elementary
School which is located in Waterboro, Maine. This elementary school is one of five elementary
schools in RSU 57. The school has a total population of 557 students in grades PreK-grade five.
The school has 42% of its students receiving free or reduced school lunch. Waterboro
Elementary School has 22.8% of its students receiving special education services. The school
does receive Title I funding. It has two reading specialists and two math specialists who
implement tier three interventions, and four education technicians who teach push in support of
Tier two plans in the classrooms.
The participants of this study were one of the four, third grade classrooms at Waterboro
Elementary School. Seventeen students in the class participated in the study. The students were
11
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
all either eight or nine years old. One student joined the study eleven days after the study began
because she had just move into the school district and had just joined the class.
Informed consent letters explaining the study, participate requirements, risks, benefits,
and confidentiality were sent to parents (Appendix F). Written consent forms were also read to
and obtained from each student who participated in this study (Appendix E).
Instruments
Multiplication Running Record Assessment by Newton (2016; Appendix B) was used as
my pre and post assessment. The biweekly informal multiplication fact quizzes and rote
memorization group practice worksheets were worksheets that the teacher has had and used for
many years (See appendix D). After reading about the characteristics of each multiple
intelligence groups strengths, I found activities that corresponded with each strength by
searching the internet and my teacher files.
The Musical-Rhythmic multiple intelligence group used multiplication raps that came
from the website Focabulary ( https://www.flocabulary.com/topics/multiplication-division/)
and the multiplication songs were from the Teacher’s Pay Teachers website (See appendix G).
The Visual Group used the Multiplication Fact Box worksheets (See appendix H). I also had my
visual group use manipulatives such as math tiles to make arrays so they could visually see the
math facts they were learning. The Kinetic-Body Group used Legos, marbles, egg cartons,
cardstock, and markers to learn their facts.
Procedure
This study began in December 2019. It began by giving each child a multiplication
running record as a pre-assessment. The running records were scored for each child and stored
to be able to compared with my post assessment data. Before each multiplication running record
12
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
was given, the project and consent form was read and explained to each student. Each student
was asked to sign the consent form if they wanted to, and all the students consented to being in
the study and signed the forms. A parent consent form was given to each student to bring home,
ask their parent to sign, and bring back to school the next day. Seventeen out of eighteen
students brought back their consent forms signed. One student did not bring back her parent
consent form, and therefore could not participate in the study because parent consent was not
given.
In December, the Multiple Intelligence Survey was presented to the class. Each
statement was read aloud to the class and students filled in how they rated themselves. The
surveys were tabulated and collected to that students could be assigned into multiple intelligence
groups.
On the following Monday, each student was given a multiplication practice worksheet to
fill out each day as part of their morning work. No instruction was given with the worksheet
other than to complete it, and the answers to the multiplication problems were listed at the top in
case the student did not know the answer and wanted to look up at the answer. Each day these
were collected as the student finished them, and on Friday they were given the multiplication test
for that times tables quiz. The percentage of correct responses were recorded.
At the beginning of week three of the intervention, the class was separated into four
groups based on how the students answered their multiple intelligence surveys. They were
separated into a Rote Memorization Group, a Visual Group, a Body-Kinesthetic Group, and a
Musical-Rhythmic Group. An area with a table and chairs was set up outside the classroom so
that groups could be pulled out for the math intervention. The rote memorization group
continued to work on their worksheets in the classroom as part of their morning work. For the
13
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
other three groups, they were called out one at a time for approximately ten minutes per day for
the math intervention. For each of the groups, activities were ready that utilized their multiple
intelligence strength. Each group was presented with what multiplication table we were working
on that week and what the multiplication strategy that went with that table was. This was
repeated each day Monday-Thursday for six weeks. At the end of each week, the formative
multiplication quiz was given.
At the end of the intervention, each student was again given the multiplication running
record, so that their pre and post multiplication running record assessments could be compared to
see what effect the intervention had on how many math facts they had learned.
Data Analysis
The data collected from this quasi-experimental study was used to determine if students
would learn more multiplication facts if they were taught their multiplication facts in a way that
utilized their multiple intelligence strength, than if they were taught through rote memorization
only. Multiplication running records were used as a pre and then post assessment to determine if
students in the multiple intelligence groups learned more facts than the group that learned their
facts by rote memorization only. The multiplication running record has twelve specifically
chosen multiplication facts for students to answer in the first part, a second part which focuses in
on their learning of their multiplication strategies, and then a short math survey. The individual
scores of each individual student were reported with the group they were in, and finally a percent
was found for each group to show the amount of growth each group had in learning their math
facts.
The amount of multiplication facts each student knew automatically out of the twelve
facts on the assessment was also totaled as when you give the multiplication running record, the
14
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
recorder scores the student on whether they could give them math fact automatically, within five
seconds (5s), or with prolonged thinking (pth). This was done to see the multiplication fluency of
each student before and after the intervention. This data was also done for each group and the
percent of growth for each group is shown. The students were not taught that they had to be
automatic in their math interventions so being automatic was not stressed in the math
intervention lessons, however, this data was collected to see if there was any difference in the
automaticity of the students between their pre and post assessments.
Results
The following tables and graphs show the details of how many facts each student learned
individually pre and post assessment. Then the results of how many math facts each group
learned by percent is shown. The results are then presented again in the same way for how many
facts each student, and then each group, learned to automaticity.
15
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Table 2
The Amount of Multiplication Facts Each Student Learned From the Intervention
Student Number
Pre-Intervention
Post Intervention
Difference
Number of facts
Number of facts
Between Pre and
answered
answered
Post Interventions
correctly
correctly
Visual-Spatial Group
1
0
4
4
3
10
11
1
7
4
7
3
10
1
8
7
Musical-Rhythmic Group
6
9
12
3
9
5
5
0
14
1
12
11
17
5
12
7
18
11
12
1
Body-Kinesthetic Group
2
7
11
4
5
5
12
7
15
10
9
-1
16
5
10
5
Rote Memorization Group
8
6
9
3
11
2
9
7
12
11
12
1
13
7
6
-1
Table 2 shows the individual student scores for the amount of multiplication facts each
student learned from the math intervention. Two students actually knew one less multiplication
fact after the intervention than before the intervention, but overall most students improved in the
amount of facts they knew after the intervention. Each of the groups had one student learn seven
more facts after the intervention then before. The Musical-Rhythmic group had the students with
the most growth. One student in that group went from knowing one fact on the pre running
16
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
record to knowing all twelve facts on the post running record. This table shows that the math
intervention appeared to make a difference in the students learning their math facts. This
becomes even more clear by looking at the percent of growth for each of the intervention groups
shown in figure 1 below.
Figure 1
Percentage of Growth from Intervention for Each Group
100%
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
32% 32%
36%
30%
21%
20%
10%
0%
Rote Memorization Visual-Spatial Body Kinesthetic Muscial-Rhythmic
Figure 1 shows that students in the multiple intelligence groups did learn more
multiplication facts than the students in the rote memorization group. The most facts were
learned by the Musical-Rhythmic Group. They had a 36% growth in learning new facts, and the
rote memorization group had the least amount of growth of new facts at 21% growth. So, the
percent of growth between the Musical-Rhythmic group, which had the highest percentage of
growth, and the Rote Memorization group, which had the lowest percentage of growth was 15%.
17
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
The mean of the three multiple intelligence groups was 32% growth while the rote
memorization group had 21% growth. The difference between these two percentages shows the
students in the multiple intelligence groups had 11% more growth than the students in the rote
memorization group. This indicates that when students are taught in a way that utilizes their
multiple intelligence strengths, they may learn more effectively.
Data was also collected in this study on the amount of multiplication facts each student
learned to automaticity. Learned to automaticity means that a student could look at a
multiplication fact written on a paper and give the answer to that fact within five seconds. If the
student got the answer correct after five seconds, it was recorded as correct, but not done to
automaticity. Table 4 shows each the amount of multiplication facts each student knew to
automaticity before and after the math intervention was done.
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
18
Table 3
The Amount of Multiplication Facts Each Student Learned to Automaticity
Student Number
Pre-Intervention
Pre-Intervention
Pre-Intervention
Number correctly
Number correctly
Number correctly
answered
answered
answered
Visual-Spatial Group
1
0
1
1
3
3
3
0
7
1
2
1
10
0
2
2
Musical-Rhythmic Group
6
4
7
3
9
1
2
2
14
1
6
5
17
5
5
0
18
6
5
-1
Body-Kinesthetic Group
2
2
5
3
5
5
5
0
15
3
7
4
16
3
5
2
Rote Memorization Group
8
2
4
2
11
1
6
5
12
6
10
4
13
4
5
1
Table 3 shows the amount of facts each student learned to automaticity compared to how
many facts the students learned, the amount of facts learned to automaticity was much less. One
student learned one less multiplication fact after the intervention than before and three students
learned did not learn any more multiplication facts after the intervention than before the
intervention. This shows that this type of math intervention may not be as effective if teaching
students to learn their multiplication facts to automaticity.
19
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Figure 4
Percent of Growth by Group for the Amount of Multiplication Facts Learned and for the Amount
of Multiplication Facts Learned to Automaticity
100%
90%
80%
70%
60%
50%
40%
36%
32% 32%
25%
30%
21%
19%
20%
14%
9%
10%
0%
Musical-Rhythmic Body-Kinesthetic Visual-Spatial Rote Memorization
Total Amount of Facts Learned Facts Learned to Automaticity
In looking at the percentage of growth by groups learning their facts to automaticity, the
results were that the rote memorization group actually had the most growth at 25% and the
visual-spatial group had the least amount of growth at 9%, with a 14% difference between the
highest and lowest groups.
The mean of the multiple intelligence group growth for automaticity was 14% growth
while the rote memorization group was 25%, with a 9% difference between the two groups. This
shows that the students in the rote memorization group learned more facts to automaticity than
the students in the multiple intelligence groups so if students want to be learn their multiplication
facts to automaticity, rote learning will produce the most learning of facts.
20
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
In summary, the results of this study show that the students in the multiple intelligence
groups learned more facts than the rote memorization group, however, the students in the rote
memorization group learned more facts to automaticity than the multiple intelligence groups did.
Discussion
In this study, the effect of teaching third grade children their multiplication facts in four
different ways was explored to determine if teaching students their multiplication facts in a way
that utilized their multiple intelligence strength would be a more effective way to have students
learn their multiplication facts than through rote memorization. A multiple intelligence survey
was given to the class to determine which group each student should be placed into. The students
were then placed into three different multiple intelligence groups (a visual-special group, a
musical-rhythmic group, a body-kinesthetic group), and a rote memorization group. Specific
activities were designed and taught to each group for a six-week math intervention. A pre and
post intervention multiplication running record assessment was given to collect data on how
many multiplication facts each student learned and how many multiplication facts each student
learned to automaticity. The data was then compiled and is shown in the tables and figures of this
study.
Limitations
One limitation of this study was students all had different background knowledge of
multiplication. The students were not assigned by any other factors other than their multiple
intelligence strengths. Some students in each group knew how to multiply, and some students
had very little exposure to multiplication. For example, the background of each student or what
students were doing with their parents at home could not be controlled. One student in the rote
21
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
memorization group was practicing her multiplication tables with flash cards with her parents at
home, and some students had no practice going on at home.
Another limitation of this study was the amount of time that the study was able to be
done in. The math intervention had to be done in the six-week time period. The results may have
been different if the intervention time could have been longer.
The final limitation was that the participants were not randomly selected; they were
selected based on their ability to participate in the study. The sample was convenient to the
researcher and may not be a representative of the total population of third graders.
Implications for Practice
Given the study results, there are several implications for practice. When giving the post
assessment for this study, the students were still using the strategies they were taught while
taking their post assessment. For example, the musical-rhythmic group was singing the songs
they had learned, the visual-spatial group was drawing arrays, pictures and sets, the body-
kinesthetic group was in motion as they gave their answers. All of the multiple intelligence
activities were not taught with speed in mind. So, it was not unexpected that those students did
not have the most growth in automaticity but did have the most learning of facts. The group who
had only practiced rote memorization was used to just knowing a fact and writing it down. They
learned in a way that was pure memorizing from writing and seeing the facts, so that was very
similar to the multiplication fact assessment they were given. Therefore, it was not unexpected to
see that they had the learned the most multiplication facts to automatically.
From the data collected and displayed in the tables and graphs of this study, it can be
concluded that the math intervention of teaching students to their multi-intelligence strengths
appeared to make a difference in the amount of multiplication facts the students were able to
22
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
learn. In comparing the group of students who only learned their multiplication facts through rote
memorization with the groups that learned their multiplication facts through a multiple
intelligence strength, the data showed the students in the multiple intelligence groups learned
more facts. The results showed the students in the rote memorization group actually improved
the most in the number of facts known to automaticity. This may be because the assessment was
most similar to the way they were learning their facts.
These results show that students should be taught what multiple intelligence is and be
able to explore what their multiple intelligence strengths are, so they have the opportunity to
strengthen ones they are weaker in, and utilize the ones they are stronger in. This could also be a
reason that providing students choice is so important when they are learning. By providing
students a choice as to how they want to learn and present their findings, they are allowed to
utilize their multiple intelligence strengths in effective ways.
Implications for research
This study had to end because the intervention time had ended, but important information
could be learned by doing a follow up assessment another six weeks or more after the post
assessment to see if there is a difference in the amount of multiplication facts that each group
retained. It would be interesting to see if the multiple intelligence groups were still using the
strategies they were taught and had retained their facts, or had they forgotten them. Did one
group have more retention of facts than the others? Have they become more automatic with their
facts because they now have had time to practice them and become more proficient at them? It
would be interesting to see if the rote memorization group still remembered the facts they had
memorized, or did they forget them because they were no longer writing them out.
23
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
A result not shown in the data because it was unexpected, and so data was not collected
on it, was the motivation and excitement to learn multiplication facts that was seen from the
multiple intelligence groups compared with the rote memorization group. The multiple
intelligence group students were waiting at the door for their math intervention time, while the
rote memorization group would ask if they really had to finish the worksheets they were given.
Four students who complete daily behavior charts because they routinely have a hard time
following class rules, did not have even one problem with following class rules the entire six-
week time of the intervention. This showed that allowing students to learn new material in a way
that utilized their multiple intelligence strength was motivating to students. Since such positive
behavior responses were noticed when teaching these groups, it would be an interesting study to
track the behavior of students who are being taught in a way that utilizes their multiple
intelligence strength versus those same students being taught in a way that makes using their
multiple intelligence strength hard to use.
Conclusion
According to Berrett and Carter (2017) math fact fluency is foundational for later
mathematics education. Students need to learn their math facts to help them be able to solve
more difficult problems. The results of this study support the idea of teaching students using their
multiple intelligences as often as possible. This study provides strong evidence that students do
learn more multiplication facts when they are given the opportunity to learn them in a way that
utilizes their multiple intelligence strength. It is recommended that more research be conducted
to determine if teaching multiplication facts in a way that utilizes multiple intelligence strengths
will help students to retain more multiplication facts, if teaching in this way could be applied to
teaching many other subjects, or if it makes a difference in student behavior in class.
24
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Reference List
Adams, F. (2013). The multiplication songs. Teachers Pay Teachers.
https://www.teacherspayteachers.com/FreeDownload/The-Multiplication-Songs-523630
Bay-Williams, J. M., & Kling, G. (2014). Enriching addition and subtraction fact mastery
through games. Teaching Children Mathematics, 21(4), 238. https://doi.org/
10.5951/teacchilmath.21.4.0238
Berrett, A. N. & Carter, N. J. (2018). Imagine math facts improves multiplication fact fluency in
third-grade students. Journal of Behavioral Education, 27 (2), 223-239.
https://doi:10.1007/s10864-017-9288-1
Bliss, S. L., Saecker, L., Sager, K. E., Mccallum, E., Rowland, E., & Skinner, C. H. (2006).
Increasing multiplication fluency using a taped-problems time-delay
intervention. PsycEXTRA Dataset, 35(3), 419–434. https://doi: 10.1037/e513792007-001
Boaler, J. (2015, January 28). Fluency without fear: Research Evidence on the best ways to learn
math facts. youcubed. https://www.youcubed.org/evidence/fluency-without-fear/
Burns, M. K., Ysseldyke, J., Nelson, P. M., & Kanive, R. (2015). Number of repetitions required
to retain single-digit multiplication math facts for elementary students. School
Psychology Quarterly,30(3), 398-405. https://doi:10.1037/spq0000097
Carr, M., Taasoobshirazi, G., Stroud, R., & Royer, J. M. (2011). Combined fluency and
cognitive strategies instruction improves mathematics achievement in early elementary
school. Contemporary Educational Psychology, 36(4), 323-333.
https://doi:10.1016/j.cedpsych.2011.04.002
Caviola, S., Gerotto, G., & Mammarella, I. C. (2016). Computer-based training for improving
mental calculation in third- and fifth-graders. Acta Psychologica, 171, 118-127.
25
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
https://doi:10.1016/j.actpsy.2016.10.005
Creswell, J. W., & Guetterman, T. C. (2019). Educational research: planning, conducting, and
evaluating quantitative and qualitative research. New York, NY: Pearson.
Candler, L. (2015) Multiple intelligence theory for kids. Teachers Pay Teachers.
https://www.teacherspayteachers.com/Product/Multiple-Intelligences-Survey
-for-Kids-Free-200841
Gardner, H. (2011). The Unschooled Mind: How Children Think and How Schools Should Teach
Basic Books.
Godfrey, C. J., & Stone, J. (2013). Mastering fact fluency: Are they game? Teaching
Children Mathematics, 20(2), 96. https://doi:10.5951/teacchilmath.20.2.0096
Greene, I., Tiernan, A. M., & Holloway, J. (2018). Cross-age peer tutoring and
fluency-based instruction to achieve fluency with mathematics computation
skills: A randomized controlled trial. Journal of Behavioral Education, 27(2),
145-171. https://doi:10.1007/s10864-018-9291-1
Math Multiplication and Division.(n.d). Flocabulary by Nearpod. Retrieved February 21, 2020
from https://www.flocabulary.com/topics/multiplication-division/
Miller, K. C., Skinner, C. H., Gibby, L., Galyon, C. E., & Meadows-Allen, S. (2011). Evaluating
generalization of addition-fact fluency using the taped-problems procedure in a
second-grade classroom. Journal of Behavioral Education, 20(3), 203–220. https://doi:
10.1007/s10864-011-9126-9
Musti-Rao, S., & Plati, E. (2015). Comparing two class wide interventions: Implications of
using technology for increasing multiplication fact fluency. Journal of Behavioral
Education, 24(4), 418–437. https://doi: 10.1007/s10864-015-9228-x
26
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Nelson, P. M., Burns, M. K., Kanive, R., & Ysseldyke, J. E. (2013). Comparison of a math fact
rehearsal and a mnemonic strategy approach for improving math fact fluency. Journal of
School Psychology, 51(6), 659-667. https://doi:10.1016/j.jsp.2013.08.00
Newton, N. (2016). Math Running Records in Action. Eye on Education Books.
Newton, N. (2016). Math Running Records. Math Running Records.
https://www.mathrunningrecords.com
O’Connell, S. & SanGiovanni, J. (2014). Mastering the Basic Math Facts in Multiplication and
Division: Strategies, Activities & Interventions to Move Students Beyond Memorization.
Heinemann.
Poncy, B. C., Fontenelle, S. F., & Skinner, C. H. (2013). Using detect, practice, and repair (DPR)
to differentiate and individualize math fact instruction in a class-wide setting. Journal of
Behavioral Education, 22(3), 211–228. https://doi: 10.1007/s10864-013-9171-7
Schutte, G.M., Duhon, G.J., Solomon, B. G., Poncy B. C., Morre, K., & Story, B. (2015). A
comparative analysis of massed vs. distributed practice on basic math fact fluency
growth rates. Journal of School Psychology, 53(2), 149-159. https://doi:
101016/j.jsp.2014.12.003
27
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Appendices
Appendix A
Multiple Intelligence Survey for Children
!
! ! !
!
!
!
!
! ! ! !
! ! ! !
! ! ! !
! ! ! !
! ! ! !
! ! ! !
!
28
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Appendix B
Multiplication Running Record
MULTIPLICATION RUNNING!
RECORD
Student!Page
0! x 4 4! x 8
1! x 2 6! x 7
5! x 3 4! x 4
10! x 7 7! x 8
2! x 6 8! x 5
3! x 9 9! x 6
Math !Running! Records!by!Dr.!Nicki!Newton!2016 © Routledge!!
!
! ! !
!
29
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Part 1:
Multiplication Running Record Recording Sheet
Strategy Levels and Accuracy
0 x 4
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
M0
0 1 2 3 4
1 x 2
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
M1
0 1 2 3 4
5 x 3
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
M5
0 1 2 3 4
10 x 7
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
M10
0 1 2 3 4
2 x 6
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
M2
0 1 2 3 4
3 x 9
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
M3
0 1 2 3 4
4 x 8
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
M4
0 1 2 3 4
6 x 7
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
M6
0 1 2 3 4
4 x 4
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
MD
0 1 2 3 4
7 x 8
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
MHN/M7
0 1 2 3 4
8 x 5
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
MHN/M8
0 1 2 3 4
9 x 6
a ca sc asc
fco/skf coh pth dk
MHN/M9
0 1 2 3 4
Codes:
a- automatic
sc- self corrected
asc- attempted self-correction
ca – counted all on fingers
skf – skip counted on fingers
coh- counted on in head
pth – prolonged thinking time
dk – didn’t know
Codes:
M0 – multiplying by 0
M1 – multiplying by 1
M10 – multiplying by 10
M5 – multiplying by 5
M2 multiplying by 2
M3 – multiplying by 3
M4
- multiplying by 4
M6 – multiplying by 6
M7 multiplying by 6
M8 multiplying by 6
M9 multiplying by 6
MD – multiplying doubles
MHN- multiplying higher
numbers
Codes:
0 – doesn’t know
1 – counting strategies
by ones or skip
counting using fingers
or drawings
2 - mental math/solving
in head (usually skip
counting)
3- using known facts
and strategies
4- automatic recall
Comments:
Math !Running! Records!by!Dr.!Nicki!Newton!2016 © Routledge!!
!
! ! !
!
!
30
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Part 2:
Multiplication Flexibility Assessment
Teacher: We are now going to administer Part II of the Running Record. In this part of the Running Record we are going to talk
about what strategies you use when you are solving basic multiplication facts. I am going to tell you a problem and then ask you to
tell me how you think about it. I am also going to ask you about some different types of facts. Take your time as you answer and
tell me what you are thinking as you see and do the math. I am going to take notes so I can remember everything that happened
during this Running Record.
Multiplying by 0
What do you do when
you are multiplying by
zero?
For example:
1 x 0
5 x 0
Multiplying by 1
What do you do when
you are multiplying by
1?
For example:
3 x 1
12 x 1
Multiplying by 10
What do you do when you
are multiplying by 10?
For example:
8 x 10
10 x 10
Multiplying by 5
What do you do when you
are multiplying by 5?
For example:
7 x 5
4 x 5
M0 M1 M10 M5
Multiplying by 2
What do you do when
you are multiplying by
2?
For example:
2 x 4
2 x 9
M2
Multiplying by 4
What do you think and
do when you are
multiplying by 4?
For example:
4 x 2
4 x 9
M4
Multiplying by 8
If I didn’t know 8 x 3 what
is a way that I could solve
this problem?
How about 8 x 9?
M8/MHN
Multiplying by 3
What strategies do you use
when you are multiplying
by 3?
For example:
3 x 3
3 x 6
M3
Multiplying by 6
What do you think and
do when you are
multiplying a number by
6?
For example:
6 x 5
6 x 9
Multiplying by 9
If I didn’t know 9 x 4,
what is a way I could
think about and solve
this problem?
Multiplying by 7
If I were stuck on 7 x 9,
what would you tell me to
do?
How about:
7 x 2 or 7 x 3?
Multiplying by doubles
What do you do think and
do when you are
multiplying a number by
itself?
For example:
5 x 5
8 x 8
M6/MHN M9/MHN M7/MHN MD
Comments/Notes about gestures, behaviors,
remarks:
Question Prompts:
That’s interesting/fascinating: tell me what you did.
That’s interesting/fascinating: tell me how you solved it.
That’s interesting/fascinating: tell me what you were thinking.
How did you solve this problem?
Can you tell me more about how you solve these types of
problems?
What do you mean when you say ________? (i.e. ten
friends/neighbor numbers etc.)
Math !Running! Records!by!Dr.!Nicki!Newton!2016 © Routledge!!
31
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Appendix C
Example of Rote Memorization worksheet for students to use when practicing their math
facts.
32
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Appendix D
Example of untimed multiplication facts quiz
33
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Appendix E
Student Consent Form
Hello,
My name is Mrs. Nugent, and I am a student at the University of Maine at Farmington. I am
interested in learning about how students learn their multiplication facts so I came up with a
project/study where I will teach students their multiplication facts in different ways to see if one
way works better than another way.
If you agree to be in my study/project, I am going to meet with you in a small group during your
math class for 10 minutes each day to work on learning your multiplication facts.
You can ask questions about this study/project at any time. If you decide at any time not to
finish, you can tell me you want to stop. This does not count for a grade in class, but your teacher
will see how many multiplication facts you have learned during the time I have been working
with you. When I present my project/study, I will not tell anyone’s name so no one will know
how you did.
If you sign this paper, it means that you have read/listened to this and that you want to be a part
of my project/study. If you don’t want to be in the project/study, don’t sign this paper. Being in
the project/study is up to you, and no one will be upset if you don’t sign this paper or if you
change your mind later.
Contact Information: If you have any questions about this study, please contact me, Julie
Nugent, at [email protected], 207-247-6126. You may also reach the faculty advisor, Brian
Cavanaugh on this study at [email protected] or 1-207-778-7385.
You may also contact the Chair of the IRB Karol Maybury at [email protected]
Your signature: _______________________________________________ Date _____________
Your printed name: ____________________________________________ Date _____________
Signature of person obtaining consent: ____________________________ Date _____________
Printed name of person obtaining consent: _________________________ Date _____________
__________________________________
_____________________________________ ________________
12-9-19
Dear Parents,
I, Julie Nugent, am inviting your child to participate in a research project. I am currently a math specialist at Waterboro Elementary School, and
I am also a student at the University of Maine at Farmington. I am researching how students best learn their multiplication facts.
What Will Your Child Be Asked to Do?
If you consent for your child to participate, your child will
Be given a multiple intelligence survey to see what their multiple intelligence strength is. (Everyone has one)
Be taught their multiplication facts in a small group for 10 minutes per day as part of their math class for 6 weeks.
Students will be grouped according to their multiple intelligence strength. Students will then be taught their multiplication facts in
ways that utilizes their multiple intelligence strength or by a rote multiplication method.
Be monitored during that 6-week period to see how many multiplication facts they have learned.
Risks
There is the slight possibility that your child may not like the other students they have been grouped with.
Your child may find they don’t like being pulled out of class to be working in a small group
Benefits: Your child may learn more about what their multiple intelligence strength is and how they can use that strength to help themselves
when learning new material. They may learn many of their multiplication facts which is a 3
rd
grade standard for our school and which will
benefit them as they continue to have to solve more complex math problems. Additionally, this study may help future students at school and in
other classrooms, as I hope to learn more about how students best learn their multiplication facts and share that information with other teachers.
Confidentiality: I will be assigning each child a number and information will be recorded only with that number attached to it. No data will be
collected using your child’s name. The list of what child goes with what number will be kept confidential on a password protected computer.
Your child’s name will not be on any of the notes or documents. Data and any paperwork will be kept in in a locked file cabinet. Your child’s
name or other identifying information will not be
reported in any publications. I will review this information with your child’s classroom teacher. She will know which student is which number
so she can use this information in her classroom instruction. This information will be shared as my capstone project at the University of Maine at
Farmington without any names attached to it. The information from this study will be destroyed at the end of the 2019/2020 school year.
Voluntary: Participation is voluntary. If you choose to have your child take part in this study, s/he may stop at any time. Whether or not your
child participates will not impact your child’s relationship with the school, his classroom teacher or any other teachers. Your child may skip any
questions he does not wish to answer or withdraw from the research project at any time.
Contact Information: If you have any questions about this study, please contact me, Julie Nugent, at [email protected] or 207-247-
6126. You may also reach the faculty advisor, Brian Cavanaugh on this study at [email protected] or 1-207-778-7385. You may also
contact the Chair of the IRB, Karol Maybury at [email protected]
Your signature below indicates that you have read and give consent for your child to participate in this project. You will receive a copy of this
form.
Please print your child’s name
Signature Date
34
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Appendix F
Parental Consent Form
35
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Appendix G
Multiplication Songs
The Multiplication Songs
Two Times Tables
(Tune: Happy Birthday)
Two, Four, Six, Eight, Ten, Twelve...
Fourteen, Sixteen, and Eighteen...
Twenty, Twenty-Two, Twenty-Four-r-r...
These are the two's up to Twelve...
But there are more....
Three Times Tables
(Tune: Yankee Doodle)
Three, Six, Nine, Twelve, Fifteen, Eighteen,
Twenty-One, Twenty-Four-r,
Twenty-Seven, Thirty, Thirty-Three, and
Thirty-Six,
The End!
Four Times Tables
(Tune: Row, Row, Row Your Boat)
Four, Eight, Twelve, Sixteen,
Twenty...Twenty-Four
Twenty-Eight, Thirty-Two, Thirty-Six, Forty,
Forty-Four and Forty-Eight (Clap, clap)
Five Times Tables:
(Normal sing-song-chant everyone uses)
5, 10, 15, 20, 25, 30,
35, 40, 45, 50, 55, 60,
Hint: Notice, each number ends
in either a 5 or 0.
2
3
4
5
The Multiplication Songs
Six Times Tables:
(Twinkle, Twinkle Little Star)
Six, Twelve, Eighteen, Twenty-Four,
Thirty, Thirty-Six, Forty-Two,
Forty-Eight and Fifty-Four,
Sixty, Sixty-Six, Seventy-Two,
Sixes are not hard to count,
We're all done,
So take a bow.
Seven Times Tables:
(Tune: The Ants Go Marching)
Seven, Fourteen, Twenty-One,
Twenty-Eighty,Thirty-Five,
Forty-Two and Forty-Nine,
Fifty-Six, Sixty-Three,
Seventy and Seventy-Seven,
Eighty-Four will end the song...
Of our seven times tables,
So!
All our ants!
Can sleep well for the night!
Do, Do, Do, Do,
Do, Do, Do, Do, Zzzzzz!!!!
Eight Times Tables:
(Tune: B-I-N-G-O)
Eight, Sixteen, and Twenty-Four,
Thirty-Two and Forty,
Forty-Eight, Fifty-Six,
Sixty-Four, Seventy-Two,
Eighty and Eighty-Eight,
And Ninety-Six will end this song!
Ole!!!
(
For added fun, run through the real song one time
before each new round of the math version until you
are just humming the letters for "B-I-N-G-O" during
the last round.)
6
7
8
Copyright (c) 2013 ~ Created by Faye Adams
4
Copyright (c) 2013 ~ Created by Faye Adams
5
Array Picture of Sets
Multiplication
Fact
Area Model Repeated Addition Equation
36
MULTIPLICATION FACT FLUENCY AND MULTIPLE INTELLEGENCES
Appendix H
Multiplication Fact Box
Multiplication Fact Box